Indian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology

Print ISSN: 2395-1443

Online ISSN: 2395-1451

CODEN : IJCEKF

Indian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology (IJCEO) is open access, a peer-reviewed medical journal, published quarterly, online, and in print, by the Innovative Education and Scientific Research Foundation (IESRF) since 2015. To fulfil our aim of rapid dissemination of knowledge, we publish articles ‘Ahead of Print’ on acceptance. In addition, the journal allows free access (Open Access) to its content, which is likely to attract more readers and citations of articles published in IJCEO. Manuscripts must be prepared in more...

  • Article highlights
  • Article tables
  • Article images

Article statistics

Viewed: 222

PDF Downloaded: 77


Get Permission Mondal, Ghoshal, Karmakar, Verma, and Kumar Bhardwaj: Accommodation and vergence functions among children with dyslexia: A review of literature


Introduction

Dyslexia is a learning disorder that affects reading, writing, and spelling skills. It is estimated to affect around 5 to 10% of the population and often runs in families. Dyslexia is a neurological condition affecting how the brain processes written language. On the other hand, binocular vision refers to using both eyes in coordination to form a single, three-dimensional image. It is an essential aspect of visual perception, and it helps us to perceive depth and distance accurately.1 Itaffects many factors, including eye movements, eye alignment, and the ability of the brain to fuse images from both eyes into a single, coherent picture.2

Research has shown that there may be a connection between dyslexia and binocular vision problems. Research suggests that people with dyslexia may also struggle with binocular vision, such as eye tracking, eye teaming, and convergence.3 These difficulties may contribute to the reading difficulties experienced by individuals with dyslexia. However, the association between dyslexia and binocular vision problems is complex, and significant research is required to understand it fully.4 Some researchers have suggested that binocular vision problems may be a consequence of dyslexia rather than a cause. In contrast, others have proposed that underlying neurological factors may cause both conditions.5 Despite the lack of consensus, it is clear that addressing binocular vision problems can improve reading performance in some individuals with dyslexia, and this area continues to be an active area of review of literature interest.

Accommodation & vergence function in children with dyslexia

Inspected investigations revealed poor monocular accommodative amplitudes and the binocular accommodative capability, which is optical power it can achieve by changing its focus. Low Negative relative accommodation (NRA) and positive relative accommodation (PRA), where NRA Measures the most outstanding accommodation relaxing capacity while preserving unobstructed, single-binocular vision and PRA Measures the most remarkable accommodation-stimulating capacity while preserving clear, single-binocular vision values, which are categorized as accommodative insufficiency which in turn results in asthenopic symptoms. Symptom reduction and improved focus may be achieved with therapy, including lens adjustments and accommodative training.6, 7 Decreased vergence reserve amplitude at near, demonstrating reduced distance base-in (NFV) vergence, inadequate vergence control, unstable binocularities, and restricted divergence at both far and near distances.8, 9, 10 Divergence deficits can occur irrespective of convergence and accommodation relaxation, and their appearance at a distance suggests that they do. The physiological studies showing different convergence and divergence modulations at the cortical and subcortical premotor levels support this unexpected finding. Many studiesreveal vergence deficiencies, typically prevalent in people with dyslexia. So, the treatment should specifically target the convergence and divergence subsystems.10 Motor deficiencies are directly related to dyslexia-related functions.11

For those who have dyslexia, these deficiencies will make life more difficult, but they are also easily remedied. Eye care professionals should take several measurements when examining people with dyslexia since it is crucial to spot refractive, accommodative, or binocular irregularities in populations with dyslexia.12 The findings confirm that dyslexia's phonological deficit are not its underlying cause rather is the cause of the recent discoveries of binocular impairments in dyslexic children.2

Therefore, the eye care practitioner must perform a binocular vision evaluation on all children, especially those with reading difficulties. In order to ensure proper diagnosis and treatment for school-aged children who have been identified as having a reading problem, a comprehensive eye examination should include tests for binocular vision and accommodative status, such as near point of convergence (NPC), accommodative facility, amplitude of accommodation, and fusional ranges.13

Objectives

Clinicians and educators need to understand how to diagnose and treat dyslexia and associated vision problems. This review article can provide valuable insights into practical assessment and intervention strategies by summarizing the latest research on these topics. Additionally, this review article can help to identify areas where more research is needed. While there has been some research on the connection between dyslexia and binocular vision, much is still not fully understood. A review article can guide future research efforts by highlighting these gaps in knowledge. Overall, a review article on dyslexia and binocular vision can be a valuable resource for researchers, clinicians, educators, and anyone interested in understanding these conditions and how they may be related.

Rational

This review paid much attention to the literature about binocular vision parameters in kids with dyslexia worldwide. A study has yet to present an all-encompassing perspective together. Nevertheless, with the assistance of a thorough literature study, we will provide an all-encompassing perspective on the binocular vision parameters in children with dyslexia and its significance in clinical practice in this review.

Materials and Methods

In this part, we will cover the approach used to conduct a literature review to investigate the accommodative and vergence parameters among children with dyslexia.

Eligibility criteria

The literature associated with any binocular vision parameters in children with dyslexia around the globe was given a significant amount of focus in this study. The approach involved examining papers from conferences as well as journal articles, and it also involved initial studies that showed alterations in binocular vision parameters. Our search focused on articles that address visual acuity, stereoacuity, accommodative and vergence parameters in dyslexic children, as these are crucial components of adequate vision during reading. Studies on dyslexia-related psychological and behavioral alterations, however, were not included. Articles in all languages were considered and translated into English using autoML translation. Much research done and published during 1991 to 2022 on visual deficits and dyslexia were used to inform this study and the age group of the children were considered between 6 -15 years.

Search strategy

Original and review papers were searched on research repositories including PubMed, Ovid, and Google Scholar. Visual deficits, accommodative inertia, the amplitude of accommodation, near point of accommodation, near point of convergence, fusional vergence, and stereopsis in children with dyslexia were keywords that were included throughout the search. Based on the title and abstract, an initial evaluation was conducted. Selected pieces were thoroughly examined before being included.

Data extraction

The inclusion applied was binocular vision parameters among children with dyslexia, and the studies related to syndrome and learning disabilities other than dyslexia, disease, and dyslexia, as well as studies with no baseline screening, were excluded. Relevant titles and abstracts were examined. The selected studies were evaluated using a data extraction form that included variables like first author, country, age, gender, and characteristics of dyslexia patients. Outcomes were categorized based on visual criteria evaluations.

Data synthesis

To provide a qualitative analysis of the included study's findings and components, the information acquired from the relevant research is summarized in the form of tables manually. Investigations of Accommodation & Vergence function in the dyslexic and typical children were compared. Literature management program, Mendeley was used for the evaluation of each study's excellence.

Study risk of bias assessment

The quality of the included papers was assessed using the ROBINS-I approach for non-randomized trials.14 Within this approach, the evaluation covered seven distinct themes: confounding, participant selection for the study, classification of interventions, deviations from intended interventions, missing data, assessment of outcomes, and selection of reported results. These themes were thoroughly examined to ensure a comprehensive analysis of potential biases, all while maintaining originality in the write-up.

Results

Study selection and characteristics.

Selection process

One thousand two hundred-five articles were identified in the initial search strategy for all keywords. Twenty-three duplicates and 562 articles were removed for other reasons, and 620 articles were screened. The abstract of 620 articles was inspected, out of which 548 were excluded, and the intention for exclusion is mentioned in the flow diagram strictly following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Figure 1 ). 15 All 18 articles were focused on and included. All the Study Design, Characteristics and Risk of bias assessment are summarized in (Table 1).

Diagram 1

The flow diagram illustrates the selection process of studies investigating binocular Vision's baseline parameters among children with dyslexia

https://typeset-prod-media-server.s3.amazonaws.com/article_uploads/abeb6bd1-0811-4839-9244-5390b9999a10/image/12d048e0-3021-4b21-b6de-3006653b8d42-uimage.png

Result outcomes

Effect on visual acuity

Visual acuity, when compared between dyslexic and normal child population studies showed no significant differences in distance visual acuity.12, 2, 13, 3, 16 Whereas studies showed a notable distinction in the near visual acuities of the compared groupswhich has been summarized in (Table 2).12, 3, 17

Table 1

Study design, characteristics and risk of bias assessment

Author

Year

Age

Sample size

Study design

Limitation

Country

ROBINS-I

Borsting et al. 6

2003

8–15

392 Typical Readers

Cross-sectional study

It is important to evaluate the matched group taking into account other factors such as screen time.

California

High

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al. 8

2010

8-13

87 Poor readers 32 Controls

Cross-sectional study

The study should assess the impact of vision therapy on reading tasks for children with poor reading skills.

Spain

Moderate

Buzzelli, Andrew R. 11

1991

13

13 Dyslexics 13 Normal readers

Cross-sectional study

…………………………

High

Zoï Kapoula et al. 10

2007

10.7±2

57 Dyslexics 46 non-dyslexics

Cross-sectional study

Research in this field should combine orthoptic tests, visual training, and recordings of eye movements during reading before and after orthoptic training.

Paris, France

Moderate

Gro Horgen Vikesdal et al. 12

2019

9-12

17 Dyslexics 17 Controls

Experimental study

It is possible that the high prevalence of hypermetropia in the dyslexic group is due to a small sample size, which could have been affected by the inclusion of more males than females.

Norway

Moderate

Wahlberg-Ramsay et al. 2

2012

13.8 ± 1.33 14.2 ± 1.67

63 Dyslexics 60 controls

cross-sectional study

…………………………..

Sweden

Moderate

Lisa W. Christian et al. 13

2017

8.6 ± 2.3

121 Poor readers

Descriptive study

All children from different schools should be included in the study. Randomized controlled trials should be conducted to determine if treating binocular vision conditions can significantly improve children's reading ability. The study should also incorporate additional tests for saccadic eye movements, such as an eye tracker.

Canada

Moderate

Jan Ygge et al18

1993

7- 9

86 Dyslexics 86 controls

Longitudinal study

………………………….

Sweden

Moderate

Wajuihian SO et al. 3

2011

13 ± 1.42 11.9 ± 0.93

31 Dyslexic Children 31 Controls

cross-sectional study

………………………….

High

Bruce J. W. Evans et al. 17

1994

8-15

39 Dyslexic Children 43 controls

cross-sectional study

…………………………

London

High

Azam Darvishi et al. 16

2022

8.1 ± 0.8

32 children with dyslexia

Randomized, observational study

Result: Discovered a strong association between the degree of dyslexia and higher near exophoria.

Mashhad, Iran

Moderate

Aparna Raghuram et al.19

2018

7-11

29 Developmental Dyslexia 33 Typical Developing

A prospective, uncontrolled observational study

Small sample size and unmasked examiners not amenable to examiner bias or influence.

Boston

Moderate

Hayes et al.20

1998

Kindergarten

297

Randomized, Prospective study

Further study with same standardized NPC method should be implemented with Symptomology questionnaire

California

Moderate

Third grade

Sixth grade

Borsting et al.21

1999

8–13

14 (CI) 14 (Normal)

Case control study

Standardized symptom Survey or Questionnaires can be used to know the efficacy of different modes of treatments

California

Moderate

Ramsay et al. 22

2012

13.80 ± 1.33

63 children with dyslexia

cross-sectional study

…………………………..

Sweden

High

14.25 ± 1.67

60 Controls

Wolfgang Dusek et al.5

2010

6-12

810 poor readers 308 controls

Retrospective clinical study

More research is necessary on binocular vision problems in school children with poor reading skills beyond Europe.

Austria

Moderate

M-L Latvala et al. 23

1994

9-10

82 Dyslexics 84 controls

cross-sectional study

Large sample size and an extended treatment time. Furthermore, a reading comprehension test that measures actual comprehension improvement would be interesting to include.

Finland, Europe

Moderate

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al. 24

2008

8-13

87 poor readers 32 controls

cross-sectional study

It is necessary to study how accommodative treatments affect the reading performance of young readers with low skills.

Spain

Moderate

Table 2

Visual acuity among dyslexic and control group

Author

Sample size

Age (Mean±SD)

Method

Visual AcuityOD/OS/OU (Mean±SD)

P- value

Gro Horgen Vikesdal et al. 201912

27 children with dyslexia

10.4 ± 1.3

Distance Visual acuity (LogMAR) Near Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OU: 0.09 ± (0.07) OU: 0.03 ± (0.05)

>0.05(Distance)

27 Controls

10.0 ± 1.1

Distance Visual acuity (LogMAR) Near Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OU: 0.06 ± (0.08) OU: 0.01 ± (0.04)

0.029(Near)

Wajuihian SO et al. 20113

31 children with dyslexia

13 ± 1.42

Distance Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OD:0.17 ± 0.31 OS:0.20 ± 0.33

OD:0.29 (Distance)

31 controls

11.90 ± 0.93

Distance Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OD: 0.00 ± 0.24 OS: 0.00 ± 0.24

OS: 0.23 (Near)

Bruce J. W. Evans et al. 1994 17

39 children with dyslexia

9.5±2.5

Near Visual acuity (LogMAR)

Worse in the dyslexic group (Mann-Whitney U-test,)

0.0018

43 controls

9.1±1.8

Wahlberg-Ramsay et al. 201212

63 children with dyslexia

13.80 ± 1.33

Distance Visual acuity (EDTRS chart at 4 m) Near Visual acuity (EDTRS-like chart at 33 cm)

OD: 1.25 ± 0.30

>0.05 (distance and near)

OU: 1.30 ± 0.3

OD: 0.86 ± 0.10

OU: 0.95 ± 0.08

60 controls

14.25 ± 1.67

Distance Visual acuity (EDTRS chart at 4 m) Near Visual acuity (EDTRS-like chart at 33 cm)

OD: 1.20 ± 0.21

OU: 1.31 ± 0.43

OD: 0.92 ± 0.12

OU: 1.03 ± 0.10

Lisa W. Christian et al. 2017 13

121 Poor readers

8.6 ± 2.3

Distance Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OD:0.04 ± 0.11

………………

OS:0.03 ± 0.11

OU: 0.00 ± 0.08

Near Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OD:0.03 ± 0.21

OS:0.03 ± 0.20

OU:0.03 ± 0.20

Azam Darvishi et al. 202216

32 children with dyslexia

8.1 ± 0.8

Distance Visual acuity (LogMAR)

OD:0.002±0.011

OS:0.004±0.018

………………..

OU:0.002±0.011

Table 3

Stereo acuity among dyslexic and control group

Author

Sample Size

Age(Mean±SD)

Stereoacuity (Mean±SD)

P-Value

Buzzelli, Andrew R. (1991)11

13 Normal readers

13±1.9

24±8.77

0.677

13 Children with dyslexia

13±1.5

23.46±15.46

Bruce J. W. Evans et al. (1994) 17

43 Normal readers

9.5±2.5

20 (median)

>0.05

38 Children with dyslexia

9.1±1.8

25 (median)

Azam Darvishi et al. 2022 16

32 children with dyslexia

8.1 ± 0.8

Mild dyslexia: 138.33±100.56

0.998

Moderate dyslexia:57±9.48

Severe Dyslexia:106.15±78.05

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al. 2010 8

87 Poor readers

10.5±1.7

25.2±11.3

>0.05

32 Normal readers

10.2±1.5

32 23.8±8.6

Monireh Feizabadi et al. 201825

27 Children with dyslexia

10±2.5

14.8% of dyslexic group worse than 60 s of arc

0.785

40 Normal readers

10±2.5

12.5% of control group worse than 60 s of arc

Gro Horgen Vikesdal et al. 2019 12

17 Dyslexics

Children with dyslexia (Children were between 4th and 6th grade)

60.00 ± (17.82)

0.018

17 Controls

Control (Children were between 4th and 6th grade)

45.00 ± (16.27)

Zoï Kapoula et al. 200710

57 dyslexics

10.7±2

<60″ (21%)

60″ (78%)

>0.05

>60″ (1%)

46 non-dyslexics

10.7±2

<60″ (31%)

60″ (69%)

>60″ (0%)

M-L Latvala et al.199423

82 dyslexics

9±0.4

>60 seconds of arc (9.1%)

>0.05

84 controls

9±0.5

>60 seconds of arc (8.0%)

Table 4

NPC among dyslexic and control group

Author

Sample Size

Age (Mean±SD)

Method

Breakpoint/ Recovery point (Mean±SD)

P-Value

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al.20108

87 Poor readers

10.5±1.7

Penlight push-up technique (three measurements by the same examiner)

3.7±3.2 / 9.1±5.2

>0.05

32 Controls

10.2±1.5

4.3±2.3 / 7.9±3.2

Ramsay et al. 2012 22

63 children with dyslexia

13.80 ± 1.33

The Royal Air Force (RAF) rule

5.80 ± 1.85 (breakpoint)

<0.001

60 Controls

14.25 ± 1.67

6.10 ± 2.10 (breakpoint)

Jameel Rizwana Hussaindeen et al.201826

20 No NSBVA Poor readers

15 ±2.1

Push-up with the accommodative task (three measurements)

7±2.5/ 8 ±0.9

0.005

46 NSBVA Poor readers

15 ±2.1

9±2.3 / 10±2.25

Gro Horgen Vikesdal et al. 201912

17 children with dyslexia

Children were between 4th and 6th grade

Push-up with the accommodative task (three measurements)

6.47 ± (2.06) (break)

>0.05

17 Controls

Children were between 4th and 6th grade

5.56 ± (1.28) (break)

Wajuihian SO et al. 20113

31 Dyslexic Children

13 ± 1.42

Push-up with accommodative task (three measurements)

8.90 ± 5.03 cm/ 14 ± 5.88 cm

0.049 (break)

31 Controls

11.90 ± 0.93

12.60 ± 8.70 cm/ 22 ± 8.20 cm

0.06 (recovery)

Monireh Feizabadi et al. 2018 25

27 Children with dyslexia

10±2.5

push-up technique using an accommodative target (single 20/30 letter).

5.25 ± 1.36

0.33

4.95 ± 1.17

40 Controls

10±2.5

Wolfgang Dusek et al. 2010 5

801 Poor readers

9±3

Penlight push-up technique (three measurements by the same examiner)

4.74 ± 5.59 (break)

< 0.001

324 controls

9±2.5

3.41 ± 4.62 cm (break)

Zoï Kapoula et al. 200710

57 dyslexics

10±2.5

Penlight push-up technique

<6cm (break): 44% =6 cm (break):14%

<0.01

7-10cm(break):36% >10 cm(break): 6%

46 non-dyslexics

10.7±2

<6cm (break): 72% =6 cm (break):13%

7 10cm(break):15% >10 cm(break): 00.0

Aparna Raghuram et al. 2018 19

29 Developmental Dyslexics

10.3± 1.2

Push-up with accommodative target

8.00±2.6 /10.57±2.96

0 .01

33 Typically developing readers

9.4± 1.4

6.30±1.57/ 8.33±1.71

Table 5

Fusional vergence system among dyslexic and control group

Author

Sample Size

AgeMean±SD

Fusional Vergence Amplitude (Mean±SD)

P-Value

Distance

Near

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al.2010 8

87 poor readers

10.5±1.7

BI Break: 9.1±3.0

BI Recovery: 3.6±1.9

BO Blur: 14.2±6.7

BO Break: 19.0±8.3

BO Recovery: 6.0±4.1

BI Blur: 13.0±3.8

BI Break: 18.8±4.7

BI Recovery: 8.9±3.3

BO Blur: 18.8±4.6

BO Break: 26.3±7.7

BO Recovery: 12.2±7.1

(BI- break) = 0.001 (BI recovery) >0.05

32 control children

10.2±1.5

BI Break: 11.1±3.4

BI Recovery: 5.0±2.4

BO Blur: 11.4±6.0

BO Break: 17.8±6.1

BO Recovery: 7.9±3.5

BI Blur: 11.5±6.6

BI Break: 17.6±5.7

BI Recovery: 9.0±4.45

BO Blur: 18.7±7.8

BO Break: 25.1±7.2

BO Recovery: 12.4±4.8

Jan Ygge et al 1993 18

86 dyslexics

2nd & 3rd grade children

BI Break: 6.5±3

BO Break: 16.8±5.3

BI Break: 10.5±2.9

BO Break: 26.5±6.8

>0.05

86 controls

2nd & 3rd grade children

BI Break: 6.2±2.6

BO Break: 16.8±6.5

BI Break: 10.2±3.2

BO Break: 26.3±7.2

Wahlberg-Ramsay et al. 2012 2

63 children with dyslexia

13.80 ± 1.33

BI Break: 10.42 ± 4.46 BO Break: 18.05 ±8.11

BI Break: 11.41 ± 3.49

BO Break: 24.56± 8.43

>0.05

60 controls

14.25 ± 1.67

BI Break: 9.87± 3.85

BO Break: 19.00± 6.41

BI Break: 12.16 ± 4.57

BO Break: 23.84± 9.16

Lisa W. Christian et al. 201713

121 Poor readers

10±4

BI Break: 9.41 ± 5.17

BI Recovery:7.44 ± 6.68

BO Break:20.86 ± 10.31

BO Recovery:13.05 ±6.5

BI Break: 13.44 ± 6.40

BI Recovery: 9.77 ± 5.36

BO Break: 21.54 ± 12.37

BO Recovery:15.1 ±8.44

______

Aparna Raghuram et al. 201819

29 Developmental Dyslexics

10.3± 1.2

BI Break: 6.71±0.36

BI Recovery: 4.20±0.34

BO Break: 18.36±1.61 BO Recovery: 12.12±0.93

BI Break: 11.31±3.13

BI Recovery: 8.28 ±3.19

BO Break: 28.72 ±7.71

BO Recovery: 22.10± 6.25

>0.05 (Distance) 0.01 (Near)

33 Typically developing readers

9.4± 1.4

BI Break: 7.31±0.32

BI Recovery: 4.86±0.30

BO Break: 16.96±1.44 BO Recovery: 11.12±0.83

BI Break: 11.70 ±2.92

BI Recovery: 8.55±2.88

BO Break: 32.97±7.40

BO Recovery: 26.39±7.13

Bruce J. W. Evans et al. 199417

24 Dyslexic

9.5±2.5

BI Blur: 9.5±4.5

BI Break: 14.2 ±5.4

BI Recovery: 9.1 ±5.3

BO Blur: 10.9±5.4

BO Break: 15.4±6.7

BO Recovery: 9.5 ±6.5

…………….

0.0084 (Break)

Control 19

9.1±1.8

BI Blur: 14.2 ±7.4

BI Break: 16.1 ±6.2

BI Recovery: 11.6 ±7.0

BO Blur: 16.7 ±9.6 BO Break: 19.0±7.8 BO Recovery: 12.3 ±8.1

………………….

0.026 (Recovery)

Wajuihian SO et al. 20113

31 dyslexics

13 ± 1.42

BI Break:14.69 ±6.83

BI Recovery:11.72 ±6.20 BO Break: 27.06±9.25 BO Recovery: 18.76±7.96

BI Break: 11.85±5.14

BI Recovery: 8.77±4.78

BO Break: 21.60±11.62

BO Recovery: 13.35±7.45

0.49 (Distance)

31 controls

11.90 ± 0.93

BI Break: 16±3.5

BI Recovery: 12.80±3.17 BO Break: 24.16±9.75 BO Recovery: 17±6.93

BI Break:12.83 ±3.13

BI Recovery: 1032±3.35

BO Break:21.09 ±8.42

BO Recovery: 15.55±6.25

0.17 (Near)

Gro Horgen Vikesdal et al. 2019 12

17 children with dyslexia

10.4 ± 1.3

……………………………..

BI Break: 8.24 ± (3.21)

0.006

17 Controls

10.0 ± 1.1

………………………………

BI Break: 11.53 ± (3.18)

[i] Note: BI: Base In, BO: Base Out

Effects on stereo acuity

In most studies11, 17, 16, 8, 25, 10, 23 Significance was not found, but a study12 showed significant differences in the stereo acuities of dyslexic and normal children with the age group between 4th and 6th grade, but the values were still found to be in the normal range hence Dyslexia shows no significant effect on stereopsis; evidence of all the studies is compiled in (Table 3 ).

Effects on NPC

(Table 4 )showing a notable difference in many studies on NPC in dyslexic population.10, 3, 20, 21, 22, 5, 19, 26 A possible explanation for these difficulties could be that dyslexia is associated with underlying neurological and cognitive differences. These differences may include problems with visual processing and attention. However, some studies have shown no significant results.8, 12, 25

Effect on fusional vergence system

(Table 5 ) compiling the studies on fusional vergence, which can affect their ability to read and focus on near objects noted that various research.8, 12, 17, 19 has suggested that some children with dyslexia may have difficulties with fusional vergence, whereas some studies denied the same.18, 2

Effect on accommodative facility

(Table 6 ) presents a summary of the accommodative facility parameters noted in several studies. The studies by19, 3, 5, 24 found significant statistical differences in the accommodative facility. However, the study by12 did not observe any statistical differences.

Table 6

Accommodative facility among dyslexic and control group

Author

Sample Size

Age (Mean±SD)

BAF/MAF (Cycle Per Minute) (Mean±SD)

P-Value

Lisa W. Christian et al. 2017 13

121 Poor readers

8.6 ± 2.3

OD: 9.15 ± 3.24

OS: 9.53 ± 3.54

>0.05

OU: 9.35 ± 3.84

Aparna Raghuram et al. 2018 19

29 Developmental Dyslexics

10.3± 1.2

5.26±2.79/4.89±2.94

< .001 (MAF)

33 Typically developing readers

9.4± 1.4

6.97±3.24/7.27±2.52

>0.05 (BAF)

Wajuihian SO et al. 20113

31 Dyslexic Children

13 ± 1.42

6.86 ± 2.74

= 0.03

31 Controls

11.90 ± 0.93

8.85 ± 3.69

Wolfgang Dusek et al. 20105

810 poor readers

9±3

6.51 ± 3.83

< 0.01

308 controls

9±2.5

9.00 ± 3.46

Gro Horgen Vikesdal et al. 2019 12

17 Dyslexics

Children with dyslexia (Children were between 4th and 6th grade)

6.41 ± (3.61)

>0.05

17 Controls

Control (Children were between 4th and 6th grade)

7.06 ± (3.21)

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al. 200824

87 poor readers

10.5±1.7

4.9±3.1

<0.001 (MAF) < 0.05 (BAF)

32 controls

10.2±1.5

6.3±2.9

[i] Note BAF: Binocular Accommodative Facility, MAF: Monocular Accommodative Facility

Effect on amplitude of accommodation

In some studies.19, 25, 27, 5 amplitude of accommodation was observed to be significantly decreased in dyslexic group in comparison with normal child population group and contrasting results were seen in other studies where no difference was seen. 22, 17, 24, 3, 16 altogether studies are compiled in (Table 7 ).

Table 7

Amplitude of accommodation among dyslexic and control group

Author

Sample Size

Age (Mean±SD)

Amplitude of Accommodation (Mean±SD)

P-Value

Ramsay et al. (2014)22

63 children with dyslexia

13.80 ± 1.33

Monocular (OD): 12.09D ± 2.65

Binocular: 13.10D ± 2.34

>0.05

60 Controls

14.25 ± 1.67

Monocular (OD): 13.30D ± 3.24

Binocular: 14.42D ± 2.10

Bruce J. W. Evans et al. (1994)17

39 children with dyslexia

9.5±2.5

OD: (Median)14.0

> 0.10

OS: (Median) 13.9

OU: (Median)16.0

43 controls

9.1±1.8

OD: (Median)16.8

OS: (Median) 18.0

OU: (Median)20.0

Aparna Raghuram et al. 201819

29 Developmental Dyslexia

10.3± 1.2

Binocular: 10.18D ± 1.99

< .001

33 Typical Developing

9.4± 1.4

Binocular: 11.77D ± 1.42

Monireh Feizabadi et al.201825

27 Children with dyslexia

10±2.5

OD: 6.90 ± 1.23 cm (NPA)

OS:7.32 ± 1.68 cm (NPA)

0. 049

OU: 6.66 ± 1.21 cm (NPA)

40 Controls

10±2.5

OD: 5.98 ± 1.15 cm (NPA)

OS:6.23 ± 1.20 cm (NPA)

OU: 6.00 ± 1.38 cm (NPA)

Catalina Palomo-Álvarez et al. 200824

87 poor readers

10.5±1.7

Monocular AOA OD: 9.1±2.3

>0.05

Monocular AOA OS: 9.0±2.3

32 controls

10.2±1.5

Monocular AOA OD: 10.5±1.7

Monocular AOA OS: 10.5±1.8

Kristen Kerber et al. 2017[27

30 dyslexic individuals

10.29±1.17

Binocular: 10.21± 2.04

0.05

33 typically reading children

9.44±1.38

Binocular: 11.5±1.48

Wajuihian SO et al. 20113

31 Dyslexic Children

13 ± 1.42

OD: 11.98 ± 2.34 D

OS: 12.14 ± 2.15 D

>0.05

31 Controls

11.90 ± 0.93

OD: 12.87 ± 1.08 D

OS: 12.87 ± 1.16 D

Wolfgang Dusek et al. 20105

810 poor readers

9±3

OU: 12.54D ± 2.60D

< 0.001

308 controls

9±2.5

OU: 13.29D ± 2.05D

Azam Darvishi et al. 2022[16

32 children with dyslexia

8.1 ± 0.8

Mild dyslexia: OU: 8.2±2.3

Moderate dyslexia: OU: .9.8±2.6

0.934

Severe dyslexia: OU: 8±1.3

Discussion

Visual acuity

Studies have suggested that children with dyslexia may have difficulties with visual perception, specifically in terms of visual acuity or clarity of vision, such as problems with visual discrimination, spatial orientation, and visual sequencing. At both a distance of 6 meters and a near distance of 40 centimeters, the visual acuity (VA) of distance was affected in a few studies. However, most studies reported hampered near VA in the dyslexic group. This differesnce in visual acuity is because children with dyslexia tend to have a more difficult time processing visual information. 28, 1, 12, 18

Stereoacuity

The provided information offers a nuanced perspective on the association between dyslexic individuals and normal subjects, emphasizing the role of factors like stereo acuity in various studies. Most studies did not find statistically significant differences in visual characteristics between dyslexic and non-dyslexic participants, suggesting a lack of consistent distinctions. 29, 16.However, a 2019 study12 stands out by reporting a significant difference in stereo acuity for dyslexic individuals, highlighting potential variations in specific visual processing aspects. Acknowledging the importance of considering study limitations, such as sample size and methodology, is crucial. The findings underscore the need for further research to explore the implications of observed differences in stereo acuity on reading abilities and ascertain whether they are primary or secondary factors associated with dyslexia. Collaborative efforts among researchers are vital to reconcile conflicting results and enhance our understanding of the intricate relationship between dyslexia and visual processing, ultimately contributing to the development of effective interventions for individuals with dyslexia.

NPC

The mean distance from the point of convergence was found to be reduced in most of the studies for the “dyslexic” group in comparison with the control groups.10, 3, 20, 21, 22, 5, 19, 26 Similar seen in the convergence insufficient population (p= 0.027).21 The study also found that the NPC break and recovery varied considerably across children in kindergarten, third grade, and sixth grade, suggesting that the age of the dyslexic population should also be taken into account when diagnosis. One of the reasons is associated with underlying neurological and cognitive differences, including problems with visual processing and attention. 20, 30, 31, 28

Fusional vergence testing

Present review of literature has suggested that some children with dyslexia may have fusional vergence difficulties, affecting their ability to read and focus on near objects. Studies found that children with dyslexia had reduced fusional vergence amplitudes compared to typically developing children, indicating a reduced ability to maintain binocular vision while looking at near objects.8, 12, 17, 19 This difficulty in fusional vergence may contribute to visual discomfort and fatigue while reading, exacerbating reading difficulties in children with dyslexia.28 However, it is essential to note that not all children with dyslexia experience problems with fusional vergence and that many other factors can contribute to reading difficulties in these children. Treatment for dyslexia typically focuses on improving language-based skills, such as phonological awareness and decoding, rather than solely targeting fusional vergence. 31

Accommodation accuracy

Accommodative latency of what was found to be optimal represents results within the range of what is considered normal, according to Scheiman and Wick. 32 Children diagnosed with developmental coordination deficit (DCD) had significantly worse accommodative accuracy (without the use of lenses) than children who served as controls.1 According to the outcomes of the binocular facility test, it was discovered that DCD had an accommodation facility of lower quality than controls. Compared to typically developing readers, individuals with developmental dyslexia exhibit significantly lower levels of both monocular and binocular accommodative facility. 19, 1

The amplitude of accommodation was assessed using the push-down method. The Amplitude of Accommodation values were inverted to make sense in the upright position. Results for the push-up were dramatically better than those for the push-down.30 According to statistical analysis, monocular (P = 0.025) and binocularly (P = 0.013), the dyslexic group showed noticeably decreased accommodation amplitude.29, 28 The amplitude of accommodation was reduced in the dyslexic group 17 (p <0.006) binocularly as well as monocularly (p <0.0014) compared to the control group.19 The groups exhibited significant differences in both the amplitude of accommodation (F = 13.67) and monocular accommodative facility (F = 18.11) (P < .001 for both). The dyslexic group exhibited a statistically significant decrease in near point of accommodation (NPA) when assessed monocularly and binocularly. 25, 5

Implications

It is necessary to evaluate the distance and near visual acuity, but a significant effect is seen in the near visual acuity; careful examination for near acuity in dyslexic children is necessary, as it can help to know their difficulty level while performing near tasks. The near point of convergence was noted to be receded in dyslexic children. These differences could affect the ability of the eyes to coordinate when focusing on near objects, leading to problems with near tasks specifically. Consider age concerning the normative value.

Optometric therapies that address fusional vergence may be beneficial for some dyslexic children who feel visual discomfort while reading, as it has been observed that alteration in fusional vergence system in dyslexic children.The affected ability of the eyes to focus on stimuli at varying distances is reported in dyslexic children, which causes ocular discomfort, eyestrain, fatigue, vision impairment, headache, and difficulties in focusing and concerning therapies that can reduce such symptoms. One clinical implication of these findings is that an optometric specialist should examine monocular accommodative amplitude and binocular accommodative capability in children with low reading levels.

Limitations of the Evidences

In addition to the findings of a comprehensive ophthalmologic examination and a full literacy assessment, examinations of vergence, accommodation, and eye movement may be helpful in the first evaluation of children with dyslexia. When assessing people with dyslexia, consider cycloplegia. More study is needed to determine the role of binocular vision on reading comprehension, performance, and fluency. Studying the effects of orthoptic training on reading performance requires combining orthoptic exams with visual instruction and documenting eye movements before and after the intervention.

Conclusion

The present review of literature findings did not provide evidence to support the notion that dyslexic children are more susceptible to any visual condition. However, it is worth noting that some vision abnormalities were more common among persons with dyslexia compared to the control group. While specific vision deficits appeared more common in the dyslexic community than the control population, the findings on dyslexia and vision conflict. However, for children to display their full potential, any visual impairment should be identified as early as possible and suitable accommodations should be made for them. They present a comprehensive assessment of the probable relationships between dyslexia and visual factors, notwithstanding the limitations of the studies that came before them. A list of conditions that have been investigated concerning their impact on dyslexia may be helpful for eye care specialists, educators, and other professionals who work with children with dyslexia. This list can be found in the present review of literature. Thus, the present review is expected to assist eye care professionals in clinical decision making while managing children with dyslexia.

Source of Funding

The author(s) receive no financial compensation for their work on this article.

Conflict of Interest

No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed by the author(s) in connection with the research, authorship, and/or publication of this work.

Acknowledgment

We acknowledge Department of Optometry, CT University, Ludhiana, Punjab, India and Department of Optometry and Vision Science, Amity University, Gurgaon, Hariyana, India for the encouragement and Support.

References

1 

M Feizabadi E Jafarzadehpur M Akrami Accommodation, Convergence, and Stereopsis in Dyslexic SchoolchildrenMiddle East Afr J Ophthalmol2018251148

2 

A Darvishi DS Rad SBQ Atigh A Hamidi JH Shandiz AS Baghini The Relation between the Severity of Reading Disorder and Visual Functions among Children with DyslexiaTaiwan J Ophthalmol202212217883

3 

W Dusek BK Pierscionek JF Mcclelland A Survey of Visual Function in an Austrian Population of School-Age Children with Reading and Writing DifficultiesBMC Ophthalmol201010116

4 

SO Wajuihian KS Naidoo A Comparison of the Visual Status of Dyslexic and Non-Dyslexic Schoolchildren in Durban, South AfricaAfr Vis Eye Health20117012943

5 

BJ Evans N Drasdo IL Richards Investigation of Accommodative and Binocular Function in DyslexiaOphthalmic Physiol Opt1994141519

6 

C Palomo-Álvarez M C Puell Accommodative Function in School Children with Reading Difficulties. Graefe’s ArchClin. Exp. Ophthalmol200824612176974

7 

A Raghuram S Gowrisankaran E Swanson D Zurakowski DG Hunter DP Waber Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children with Developmental DyslexiaJAMA Ophthalmol201813610108995

8 

MW Ramsay C Davidson M Ljungblad M Tjärnberg R Brautaset M Nilsson Can vergence training improve reading in dyslexics?Strabismus201422414751

9 

M Goldish Everything you need to know about dyslexiaThe Rosen Publishing Group, IncNew York2000

10 

M Wahlberg-Ramsay M Nordström J Salkic R Brautaset Evaluation of Aspects of Binocular Vision in Children with Dyslexia201220139183https://doi.org/10.3109/09273972.2012.735335

11 

E N-Szwedo F Alramis LW Christian Association between Fine Motor Skills and Binocular Visual Function in Children with Reading DifficultiesHum Mov Sci2017561110

12 

E Borsting M W Rouse P N Deland S Hovett D Kimura M Park B Stephens Association of Symptoms and Convergence and Accommodative Insufficiency in School-Age ChildrenJ Optom20037412534

13 

S Abdi R Brautaset A Rydberg T Pansell The Influence of Accommodative Insufficiency on ReadingClin. Exp. Optom20079013643

14 

C Palomo-Álvarez MC Puell Binocular Function in School Children with Reading Difficulties. Graefe’s ArchClin. Exp. Ophthalmol2009248688592

15 

J Stein Neuropsychologia20034113178593https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932

16 

Z Kapoula M P Bucci F Jurion J Ayoun F Afkhami D Brémond-Gignac Evidence for Frequent Divergence Impairment in French Dyslexic Children: Deficit of Convergence Relaxation or of Divergence per Se? Graefe’s ArchClin. Exp. Ophthalmol20062457931937

17 

AR Buzzelli Accommodative and Vergence Facility: Do They Relate to Dyslexia?Optom Vis Sci199168118426

18 

GH Vikesdal M Mon-Williams T Langaas Optometric Disorders in Children and Adults with DyslexiaScand J Educ Res202064460111

19 

LW Christian K Nandakumar PK Hrynchak EL Irving Visual and Binocular Status in Elementary School Children with a Reading ProblemJ. Optom2018113160166

20 

JAC Sterne MA Hernán BC Reeves J Savović ND Berkman M Viswanathan D Henry DG Altman MT Ansari I Boutron J Carpenter A Chan R Churchill A Hróbjartsson J Kirkham P Jüni Y Loke T Pigott D Regidor H Rothstein L Sandhu P Santaguida HJ Schünemann B Shea I Shrier P Tugwell L Turner C Jeffrey H Waddington E Waters P Whiting J P Higgins ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventionsBMJ201612 October355i4919

21 

JJ Yepes-Nuñez G Urrútia M Romero-García S Alonso-Fernández P The Statement: An Updated Guideline for Reporting Systematic ReviewsRev Esp Cardiol202174979099

22 

GJ Hayes BE Cohen MW Rouse PN De Land Normative Values for the Nearpoint of Convergence of Elementary SchoolchildrenOptom Vis Sci199875750612

23 

E Borsting MW Rouse PNDe Land Prospective Comparison of Convergence Insufficiency and Normal Binocular Children on CIRS Symptom SurveysOptom Vis Sci19997642218

24 

ML Latvala TT Korhonen M Penttinen P Laippala Ophthalmic Findings in Dyslexic SchoolchildrenBr J Ophthalmol199478533943

25 

J Ygge G Lennerstrand A Rydberg S Wijecoon BM Pettersson Oculomotor Functions in a Swedish Population of Dyslexic and Normally Reading ChildrenActa Ophthalmol19937111021

26 

JR Hussaindeen P Shah KK Ramani L Ramanujan Efficacy of Vision Therapy in Children with Learning Disability and Associated Binocular Vision AnomaliesJ Optom2018111408

27 

K Kerber S Gowrisankaran EA Swanson D Waber A Raghuram Ocular Accommodation Deficits and near Work Associated Symptoms in Children with DyslexiaInvest Ophthalmol Vis Sci20175885429

28 

J Bosch-Bayard V Peluso L Galan PV Sosa GA Chiarenza Clinical and Electrophysiological Differences between Subjects with Dysphonetic Dyslexia and Non-Specific Reading DelayBrain Sci201889172

29 

C Bilbao DP Piñero Diagnosis of Oculomotor Anomalies in Children with Learning DisordersClin Exp Optom20201035597609

30 

G Schulte-Körne Diagnostik Und Therapie Der Lese-Rechtschreib-Störung Gerd Schulte-KörneDtsch Arztebl Int20101074171827

31 

SE Shaywitz BA Shaywitz JM Fletcher MD Escobar Prevalence of Reading Disability in Boys and Girls: Results of the Connecticut Longitudinal StudyJAMA J Am Med Assoc199026489981002

32 

S Mitchell W Bruce Clinical Management of Binocular Vision: Heterophoric, Accommodative, and Eye Movement Disorders4LWWUnited StatesAugust201365978-1451175257



jats-html.xsl


This is an Open Access (OA) journal, and articles are distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 License, which allows others to remix, tweak, and build upon the work non-commercially, as long as appropriate credit is given and the new creations are licensed under the identical terms.

Article type

Review Article


Article page

415-429


Authors Details

Animesh Mondal, Rituparna Ghoshal*, Sourav Karmakar, Nishtha Verma, Gaurav Kumar Bhardwaj


Article History

Received : 13-01-2024

Accepted : 15-04-2024


Article Metrics


View Article As

 


Downlaod Files